The
child is born into a family which is the product of the operations of human
beings already in this world. It is a
system mediated through sight, sound, taste, smell, touch, pain and pleasure,
heat and cold, an ocean in which the child quickly learns to swim.
(R D Laing, 1971, p.11)
This essay will critically evaluate the mechanisms involved in the
passing of trauma from parent to child using the various theories of the transmission
of trauma: with a focus on the Northern Ireland perspective (concentrating on
the impact of the ‘Troubles’); as well as international experiences (mainly, the
long term effects of the Holocaust).
Trauma, as a concept, is multicomplex and thus its impact can be
experienced in various ways by individuals, by families, by communities, and by
societies. Summerfield (2000, p.232)
posits that, ‘[t]here is no such thing as a universal response to highly
stressful events’; while Douglass and Vogler (2003, p.10) assert that ‘[…] the
pathogenic traumatic experience of one person is an interpretive construct that
may not be shared in another, even in identical situations’.
Herman (2001, p.33) characterises trauma as an affliction of the
powerless in which the victim is rendered helpless by an overwhelming force:
fundamentally altering the ‘systems of care that give people a sense of
control, connection and meaning’.
Auerhahn and Laub (1998, p.22) describe how a ‘massive psychic trauma
[can] shape the internal representations of reality’. This is supported by Van der Kolk and McFarlane
(1996, p.6) who state that, ‘[...] the core issue of trauma is reality’ and
that the ‘meaning [attached] to the [trauma] is as fundamental as the trauma
itself: leading to an inability to ‘integrate the reality’, resulting in a
‘repetitive replaying of the trauma’. Douglass
and Vogler (2003, p.42) summarised Freud’s speculation that in cases where the
traumatic event is so extreme one does not in fact experience it - i.e. it is
‘not integrated mentally and emotionally into one’s sense of being’.
There may be positive effects after trauma, such as Post Traumatic
Growth, but the majority of trauma literature focuses on the negative (Tedeschi
and Calhoun, 1996).
This essay will concentrate on the negative effects and psychopathology
of traumatic experiences and how these effects may potentially be ‘passed’ to
the offspring of those who were initially affected (Weingarten, 2004, p.45). There are multiple models put forward to
explain transmission but due to space this essay will analyse the
psychodynamic, sociocultural and family systems theories.
Research into the sequelae of the Holocaust brought the conceptual
framework of Transgenerational Trauma (TGT) transmission to the fore. Weingarten (2004, p.49) posits that the
offspring of traumatised people act as witnesses to their parents experiences
while stating that it is not the trauma itself that is passed, but ‘its
impact’; or as Lev-Wiesel (2007, p.76) puts it: ‘its contagion’. Herman (2001, p.2) states that, ‘[w]itnesses
as well as victims are subject to the dialectic of trauma’. The DSM-V-TR (American Psychological Association,
2013, p.271)
criteria categorises repeated or extreme indirect
exposure to ‘aversive details of the event[s] [as a stressor for PTSD diagnosis,
usually in the course of professional duties]’. This criterion omits
offspring, begging the question: Why? Why
are children, who face similar exposure, around the clock, not considered to be
affected in the same way as professionals?
Kellerman (2001, p.257), who concentrated on the long term effects
of the Holocaust, presents an integrative model to demonstrate how trauma is
transmitted and characterises the ‘process’ as a ‘functional
relationship’. This ‘content’ of this relationship
can manifest itself in a way that has an adverse pathological effect on the
offspring of survivors (Ibid., p.257). The child is liable to experience problems
with: (1) ‘Self’, which would impair
‘self-esteem’ and ‘identity’ development; (2) ‘Cognition’, promoting ‘[c]atastrophic expectancy and ‘stress upon
exposure’ [to disturbing] ‘stimuli’; (3) ‘Affectivity’,
‘[a]nnihilation, anxiety, nightmares’ [and] ‘unresolved conflicts around anger
complicated by guilt’; and (4) ‘Interpersonal
functioning’, involving ‘[e]xaggerated family attachments and dependency or
exaggerated independence’ [which may hamper] ‘intimate relationships and the ‘handling
[of] interpersonal conflicts’ (Ibid.,
p.259). McKenna (2015, p.40) postulates
‘that the children of victims are at an increased risk of experiencing toxic
stress’: which has been defined by Shonkoff et
al. (2009, p.360) as the ‘strong, frequent, and/or prolonged activation of
the body’s stress response’.
Kellerman (2001) puts forward four major theoretical models for
the transmission of TGT: Psychodynamic; Sociocultural; Family Systems; and
Biological. Downes et al. (2012, pp.
584-586), who researched families bereaved during the conflict in Northern
Ireland, summarise similar approaches including: stress-vulnerability models
(in which ‘increased vulnerability to
develop psychopathology is [...] transmitted’ [emphasis in the original]); transmission
of psychopathology (whereby ‘the parental trauma in itself is not transmitted,
but rather that the parental psychopathology
is the factor that influences the children’ [emphasis in the original]);
genetic and physiological explanations (which posits that the stressor is
‘transmitted physiologically’ to the offspring); social psychological/parenting
perspectives (which focuses on ‘social learning and effects on parenting’);
psychodynamic theories (through transposition; overidentification; role
reversal; and attachment problems); and family system approaches (with a focus
on ‘particular interpersonal patterns present in families’). Hanna et al. (2012) encapsulate a phenomenon
of poor psychological functioning in children which may stem from the impact of
trauma experienced by their caregivers, leading to a deficit in optimal
interactions.
While some of these models may be discrete, others are more
intertwined and connected. Kellerman
(2001) argues that any or all of the manifestations of trauma transmission can
be determinants. Weingarten (2004, p.49)
echoes this line stating that ‘no mechanism alone is the answer to how trauma passes; none is incontrovertible; and none
can be easily separated from the others’ [emphasis in the original].
The psychodynamic model of transmission, according to Kellerman
(2001, p. 260), comes from the psychoanalytic tradition where ‘repressed,
[unresolved], and insufficiently dealt with’ emotions within the primary
victims are passed over to the next generation through unconscious ‘absorption’:
which Hesse and Van IJzendoorn (1998, p.304) purport as having the potential to
lead to ‘substantial alterations in consciousness’. According to Volkan (1997, in Kellerman,
2001, p.260) the elder ‘unconsciously externalizes his traumatized self onto the developing child’s personality’:
an influence that the child cannot repel; thus it becomes the child’s ‘task, [to]
mourn, to reverse the humiliation [and shame]’ [emphasis added]. Lev-Wiesel (2007, p.90) also recognises this
dynamic, in that, the 2nd generation were expected to repay a ‘debt
[…] for the suffering of the 1st generation [Holocaust survivors]’:
it became their life’s ‘mission’. A
recent full page advertisement from a local victim-centred NGO in one of the
main newspapers in Northern Ireland (The Irish News) echoes this mission
mentality being placed upon subsequent generations, to perpetually seek truth
and justice for human rights violations, by stating that, ‘[The UK] Government
is waiting for us to die off but our families will not go away’ (News Letter,
2015).
Rowland-Klein and Dunlop’s (1997, p. 366-367) Holocaust research
identifies this ‘intimate, intrapsychic, and unconscious’ process in the form
of ‘projective identification’: in which the parents attempt to self-heal
through the children, who reciprocate ‘despite the cost to themselves’; and
‘whereby the parent splits off the unwanted part of the self, which is
projected into the child, and internalised by it; becoming [Kleinian] ‘containers’
for the parent [emphasis added]. DeGraaf
(1998,
in Downes et al., 2012, p. 585) describes a ’bad child’ within the parent being
externalised through ‘anger, rage, sadness disappointment and grief onto their child’ [emphasis added]. Themes that arose in Rowland-Klein and
Dunlop’s (1997, pp. 366-367) research included ‘overidentification’ and
‘re-enactment’ which involved the child placing itself in an analogous
situation to the parent where they would try to find meaning and ‘share the
suffering’, in a process of ‘introjection’ and ‘transposition’.
Overidentification and overprotectiveness are evident when
children display similar traits to the parents such as hypervigilance and a
pervasive mistrust of others; in which the world is a dangerous place and the
family is the only safe haven (Danieli, 1985).
Rowland-Klein and Dunlop, (1997, p.367) posit that this can compromise
the child’s ‘own sense of security’.
This observation is corroborated by Black’s (2004,
p.104) research into the children of police officers
in Northern Ireland, in which ‘[the children] may develop anxiety regarding
their parent’s or their own safety’. The
child becomes enmeshed in the reality of the parent in a ‘symbiotic’
relationship; boundaries are blurred; role reversal occurs (‘parental child
[vs] adult child’); potentially leaving the child with problems around ‘separation’,
‘individuation’ and ‘autonomy’ (Freyberg, 1980, p.90; Rowland-Klein and Dunlop,
1997, p.366; Downes et al., 2012,
p.593). This type of relationship can be
detrimental to both parent and child as they may become entwined in what
Karpman (1968) has described as ‘The Drama Triangle’.
Kellerman (2001, p.261) shone a light on the ‘sociocultural and
socialization models of transmission’ in which it is postulated that social
norms and beliefs are passed down from ‘generation to generation’; whereby
children ‘form their own images through their parents’ childrearing
behaviour’. This contrasts with
psychodynamic theories of the unconscious to more direct and conscious
learning. In Holocaust literature, according
to Kellerman (2001, p.261) survivor parents have been described as
‘inadequate’: as their extreme suffering was ‘assumed to create child-rearing
problems around both attachment and detachment’. Difficulties with attachment have been posited as a prominent
conceptual framework for the transmission of TGT (Liotti, 1992; Bar-on et al., 1998; Kellerman, 2001; Downes et al., 2012; McKenna, 2015). Attachment is a psychological model which
describes the bonds and interactions between a young child and its primary
caregiver and it is believed to be an important determinant of the child’s
successful social and emotional development (Commission for Victims and
Survivors, 2015, p.23). Bowlby (1982)
describes how we are born with a strong tendency to seek care, help, and
comfort in times of danger or when suffering from physical or emotional
stress. Bar-on et al. (1998, p.318) assert that ‘[...] a primary function of
attachment relationships is to serve as a source of security [...] in
situations that induce fear or anxiety’.
Ideally, in an optimal relationship, the caregiver gives a positive
response to the child: problems arise when a negative response is offered. These problems may manifest in children as
‘disorganised’ or ‘insecure-ambivalent and preoccupied attachment strateg[ies]’,
which could lead to an increased vulnerability to ‘dissociative disorders’ (Liotti,
1992, p.196; Bar-on et al., 1998, p.330).
McKenna (2015, p.37) asserts that ‘parental trauma exposures
interfere with interaction patterns within families’. Bar-on et
al. (1998, p.319) postulate that ‘parents who are unable to monitor their
discourse and thoughts [around traumatic experiences] appear to have children
who show a lack of consistent attachment strategy’; adding that it is the ‘lack
of successful coping, rather than the loss/trauma per se that contributed to this relation’. Main and Hesse (1990, in Bar-on et al., 1998, p.320) propose that this
‘lack of resolution [...] is characterised by parental fear [...] perceived by
the child as being either a frightened model or as directly frightening the
child’; and that the attachment figure is ‘at once the source and the solution
of the infant’s alarm, and this leads to a paradox of fright without
solution’.
McKenna (2015) posits that communication within families can be a
detrimental determinant of TGT transmission.
McNally (2014, p.32) asserted that traumatic experiences could lead to
the development of ‘unhealthy methods of communication’ within the family:
ranging ‘from silence to intrusive attempts to discuss the events and imposing
their interpretations [onto their children]’.
This type of communication can be either intentional or unintentional:
verbal or non-verbal (McNally, 2014).
Bergman and Jucovy (1982) signal the importance of how the narrative of
the trauma is relayed to children, in what spirit, or whether the information
is used to educate or employed as a threat.
Danieli (1985, p.298) highlights this ‘Conspiracy of Silence’ as a
major factor in the transmission of TGT.
The problems with a frightening narrative explained above are seen in
contrast to how silence around trauma can be even more insidious to the next
generation. Many of the families who
survived trauma employed silence as a way of coping with their extreme
experiences; and also to protect their children. Their Holocaust experiences were too
horrifying to recount; there were no words; they were dealing with feelings of
shame and guilt; and at the same time their audience could not or would not
listen to or believe them: which led to problems with intrapsychic integration
and healing (Ibid.). Nevertheless, many of their offspring
‘attested to the constant psychological presence of the Holocaust at home,
verbally and nonverbally [and] reported having absorbed the omnipresent
experience [...] through osmosis’ (Ibid.,
p.299).
However, Bar-on et al.,
(1998, p.331) stress that the conspiracy of silence ‘cannot be total’. The offspring may hear partial facts and use
their imagination to complete the narrative which may result in a more
pervasive interpretation; where the ‘made-up story may be even more frightening
than the real one’ (Dekel and Goldblatt, 2008, p.285). They are left with an emotional story without
an actual narrative to make sense of it.
Bar-on (1995, in Bar-on et al.,
1998, p.326) asserted that, ‘the “untold story” of the past was [transmitted]
with greater intensity [...] than the “told” story’; and that the children
became sensitive to their parents’ need to keep silent responding with a
“double wall”: [t]he parents did not tell and the children did not ask’.
Danieli (1985, pp.299-304) described four categories of
post-Holocaust survivor families to give a sense of the systems they employed:
(1) Victim families (characterised by
a victim identity, pervasive depression, worry, mistrust, fear of the outside
world, and symbiotic clinging); (2) Fighter
families (characterised by an ‘intense drive to build and achieve’,
mistrust, overinvolvement and overprotectiveness, contemptuous of dependency on
others); (3) Numb families
(characterised by ‘pervasive silence and depletion of all emotions’, role
reversal, children grew up on their own and taught themselves how to live); an
observation supported by Becker and Diaz (1998) who argue that such children
had to mature rapidly in order to become protectors of their parents; and (4) Families of ‘those who made it’
(characterised by a ‘desire to make it big’, children were emotionally
neglected, denial of impact of Holocaust).
All of these families have a commonality in that the past is ‘taboo,
excluded from open discussion, [which] cannot then become [an integrated]
history (Becker and Weyermann (2006, p.28).
Downes
et al. (2012, pp.590-595) located
this theme in their research of families affected by the NI conflict: whereby
families hid ‘aspects of the traumatic experiences and engage[d] in cognitive
and affective avoidance’; where the ‘unspoken’ was prevalent; and the children
‘invented [their] own explanations; a place where the ‘truth [was] dangerous’; where
facts were ‘hidden but not hidden’; where the children attempted to ‘block out
their feeling for fear of the consequences; upon which one child developed the
‘propensity to be the “good girl” [in] a conscious attempt to avoid causing her
own mother any more stress’. This echoes
Dekel and Goldblatt’s (2008, p.285) observations where ‘sensitive subjects are
avoided’ to prevent the intensification of stress.
In
conclusion, it becomes clear from the evidence that there is a case to be made
for the existence of mechanisms which may pass trauma from one generation to
the next; and in certain cases to subsequent unborn generations. However, they
are not mutually exclusive and they may not always pass on pathological
consequences. There can be recovery;
missions can be accomplished; and growth can occur.
The
main focus of this essay was concerned with transmission inside families however
wider society and political responses can have a profound effect on traumatic
sequelae (Becker, 2004; McNally, 2014).
From this comes a view that trauma is a biopsychosocial issue that
should not be seen through a purely pathological diagnostic lens; ‘in which the
basic focus is not so much on the symptoms […] but on the sequential
development of the traumatic situation’ (Becker, 2004, p.6). This point is echoed by McNally (2014) who
argues that TGT should not be given a clear diagnostic criterion as this may
steer any responses firmly in the direction of the medical model to the
detriment of other treatment modalities, and of the individuals and families
themselves. Until the wider issues, that
pervade societies affected by protracted conflict, violence, and trauma, are
addressed the chances of seriously dealing with the long term legacy of
traumatised families are diminished.
References
American
Psychiatric Association. (2013) Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental
Disorders. 5th edn. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Publishing.
Auerhahn,
N.C. and Laub, D. (1998) ‘Intergenerational Memory of the Holocaust’, in
Danieli, Y. (ed.) International Handbook
of Multigenerational Legacies of Trauma. New York: Plenum Press, pp. 21-41.
Bar-On,
D. (1995) Fear and Hope: Three
Generations of the Holocaust. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Bar-On,
D., Eland, J., Kleber, R.J., Krell, R., Moore, Y., and Sagi, A. (1998) Multigenerational
perspectives on coping with the Holocaust experience: an attachment perspective
for understanding the developmental sequalae of trauma across generations’, International Journal of Behavioural
Development, 22(2), pp. 315-338.
Becker, D.,
and Diaz, M. (1998) ‘The Social Process and the Transgenerational Transmission
of Trauma in Chile’, in Danieli, Y. (ed.) International
Handbook of Multigenerational Legacies of Trauma. New York: Plenum Press,
pp. 435-
445.
Becker, D.
and Weyermann, B. (2006) Toolkit: Gender,
Conflict Transformation and the Psychosocial Approach. Bern: Swiss
Development Co-operation.
Bergmann, M.S and
Jucovy, M.E. (eds.) (1982) Generations of
the Holocaust. New York: Basic Books.
Black, A. (2004) ‘The treatment of psychological problems experienced by
the children of police officers in Northern Ireland’, Child Care in Practice, 10(2), pp. 99-106.
Bowlby, J. (1982) Attachment and loss. Vol.1: Attachment.
2nd edn. London: Hogarth Press.
Commission
for Victims and Survivors (2015) Towards
A Better Future: The Trans-generational Impact of the Troubles on Mental Health.
Belfast: Commission for Victims and Survivors.
Danieli, Y.
(1985) ‘The
Treatment and Prevention of Long-term Effects and Intergenerational
Transmission of Victimization: A Lesson From Holocaust Survivors and Their
Children’, in Figley, C.R. (ed.) Trauma and its wake. Vol.1, The study and treatment of post-traumatic
stress disorder. New York: Brunner/Mazel, pp.
295-313.
DeGraaf,
T. (1998) ‘A family therapeutic approach to transgenerational traumatization’, Family
Process, 37(2), pp.
233–243.
Dekel, R. and
Goldblatt, H. (2008) ‘Is There Intergenerational Transmission of Trauma? The
Case of Combat Veterans’ Children’, American
Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 78(3), pp. 281–289.
Downes, C., Harrison,
E., Curran, D., & Kavanagh, M. (2013) ‘The trauma still goes on...: the
multigenerational legacy of Northern Ireland's conflict’, Clinical Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 18(4), pp. 583-603.
Douglass, A. and
Vogler, T.A. (eds.) (2003) Witness and Memory: The Discourse of Trauma. Routledge,
London.
Freyberg,
J.T. (1980) ‘Difficulties in Separation-Individuation as experienced by
offspring of Nazi Holocaust Survivors’, American
Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 50(1), pp. 87-95.
Hanna, D.,
Dempster, M., Dyer, K., Lyons, E. and Devaney, L. (2012) Young People's Transgenerational Issues in Northern Ireland.
Belfast: Commission for Victims and Survivors.
Herman,
J.L. (2001)
Trauma and recovery: from domestic abuse to political
terror. London: Pandora.
Hesse, E.
and Van IJzendoorn, M.H. (1998) ‘Parental loss of close family members and
propensities towards absorption in offspring’, Developmental Science, 1(2), pp.299-305.
Karpman, S. (1968)
‘Fairy tales and script drama analysis’, Transactional
Analysis Bulletin, 7(26), pp. 39-43.
Kellermann,
N.P.F. (2001) ‘Transmission of Holocaust Trauma. An integrative view’, Psychiatry: Interpersonal and Biological
Processes, 64, pp.256–267.
Laing, R.D. (1971) The Politics of the Family and Other
Essays. London: Tavistock Publications.
Lev-Wiesel,
R. (2007) ‘Intergenerational Transmission of Trauma across Three Generations: A
Preliminary Study’, Qualitative Social
Work, 6(1), pp. 75-94.
Liotti, G.
(1992) ‘Disorganized/disoriented attachments in the etiology of the dissociative
disorders’, Dissociation,
5, pp. 196-204. Main, M. and Hesse, E.
(1990) ‘Parents' unresolved traumatic experiences are related to infant 34
disorganized attachment status: Is frightened and/or frightening parental
behavior the linking mechanism?’, in Greenberg, M.T., Cicchetti, D. and E.M.
Cummings, E.M. (eds.) Attachment in the
preschool years. Chicago: Chicago University Press, pp. 161-182.
McKenna, A.
(2015) ‘The impact of the conflict’s legacy on early years’ development of children and young people’, in
Commission for Victims and Survivors, Towards
A Better Future: The Trans-generational Impact of the Troubles on Mental Health.
Belfast: Commission for Victims and Survivors, pp.
36-49.
McNally, D,
(2014) Transgenerational Trauma and
Dealing with the Past in Northern Ireland. Belfast: WAVE Trauma Centre.
News
Letter (2015) ‘Troubles victims use
newspaper ad to attack Government over legacy issues’,
[Online] Available at:
http://www.newsletter.co.uk/news/northern-ireland-news/troubles-victims-use-newspaper-ad-to-attack-goverment-over-legacy-issues-1-7108985
(Accessed: 11 December 2015).
Rowland-Klein
and Dunlop’s (1997) ‘The transmission of trauma across generations:
Identification with parental trauma in children of Holocaust survivors’, Australian and New Zealand Journal of
Psychiatry, 32(3), pp. 358-369.
Shonkoff,
J.P. (2010)
‘Building a New Biodevelopmental Framework to Guide the Future of Early
Childhood Policy’, Child Development,
81(1), pp.357-367.
Summerfield,
D. (2000) ‘Conflict and health - War and mental health: a brief overview’,
BMJ, 321, pp. 232–235.
Tedeschi, R.G. &
Calhoun, L.G. (1996). ‘The posttraumatic growth inventory: measuring the
positive legacy of trauma’, Journal of
Traumatic Stress, 9(3), pp. 455-471.
Van der
Kolk, B. and McFarlane, A.C. (1996) ‘The Black Hole of Trauma’, in Van der
Kolk, B., McFarlane, A.C. and Weisaeth, L. (eds.) Traumatic Stress: The Effects of Overwhelming Experience on
Mind, Body, and Society. New York, NY: Guilford Press, pp. 3-23.
Volkan, V.
(1997) Bloodlines: From Ethnic Pride to
Ethnic Terrorism. New York: Basic Books.
Weingarten,
K. (2004) ‘Witnessing the effects of political violence in families: Mechanisms
of intergenerational transmission of trauma and clinical interventions’, Journal of Marital and Family Therapy,
30(1), pp. 45-59.
Trauma, as a concept, is multicomplex and thus its impact can be experienced in various ways by individuals, by families, by communities, and by societies. Summerfield (2000, p.232) posits that, ‘[t]here is no such thing as a universal response to highly stressful events’; while Douglass and Vogler (2003, p.10) assert that ‘[…] the pathogenic traumatic experience of one person is an interpretive construct that may not be shared in another, even in identical situations’.
Herman (2001, p.33) characterises trauma as an affliction of the powerless in which the victim is rendered helpless by an overwhelming force: fundamentally altering the ‘systems of care that give people a sense of control, connection and meaning’. Auerhahn and Laub (1998, p.22) describe how a ‘massive psychic trauma [can] shape the internal representations of reality’. This is supported by Van der Kolk and McFarlane (1996, p.6) who state that, ‘[...] the core issue of trauma is reality’ and that the ‘meaning [attached] to the [trauma] is as fundamental as the trauma itself: leading to an inability to ‘integrate the reality’, resulting in a ‘repetitive replaying of the trauma’. Douglass and Vogler (2003, p.42) summarised Freud’s speculation that in cases where the traumatic event is so extreme one does not in fact experience it - i.e. it is ‘not integrated mentally and emotionally into one’s sense of being’.
There may be positive effects after trauma, such as Post Traumatic Growth, but the majority of trauma literature focuses on the negative (Tedeschi and Calhoun, 1996). This essay will concentrate on the negative effects and psychopathology of traumatic experiences and how these effects may potentially be ‘passed’ to the offspring of those who were initially affected (Weingarten, 2004, p.45). There are multiple models put forward to explain transmission but due to space this essay will analyse the psychodynamic, sociocultural and family systems theories.
Research into the sequelae of the Holocaust brought the conceptual framework of Transgenerational Trauma (TGT) transmission to the fore. Weingarten (2004, p.49) posits that the offspring of traumatised people act as witnesses to their parents experiences while stating that it is not the trauma itself that is passed, but ‘its impact’; or as Lev-Wiesel (2007, p.76) puts it: ‘its contagion’. Herman (2001, p.2) states that, ‘[w]itnesses as well as victims are subject to the dialectic of trauma’. The DSM-V-TR (American Psychological Association, 2013, p.271) criteria categorises repeated or extreme indirect exposure to ‘aversive details of the event[s] [as a stressor for PTSD diagnosis, usually in the course of professional duties]’. This criterion omits offspring, begging the question: Why? Why are children, who face similar exposure, around the clock, not considered to be affected in the same way as professionals?
Kellerman (2001, p.257), who concentrated on the long term effects of the Holocaust, presents an integrative model to demonstrate how trauma is transmitted and characterises the ‘process’ as a ‘functional relationship’. This ‘content’ of this relationship can manifest itself in a way that has an adverse pathological effect on the offspring of survivors (Ibid., p.257). The child is liable to experience problems with: (1) ‘Self’, which would impair ‘self-esteem’ and ‘identity’ development; (2) ‘Cognition’, promoting ‘[c]atastrophic expectancy and ‘stress upon exposure’ [to disturbing] ‘stimuli’; (3) ‘Affectivity’, ‘[a]nnihilation, anxiety, nightmares’ [and] ‘unresolved conflicts around anger complicated by guilt’; and (4) ‘Interpersonal functioning’, involving ‘[e]xaggerated family attachments and dependency or exaggerated independence’ [which may hamper] ‘intimate relationships and the ‘handling [of] interpersonal conflicts’ (Ibid., p.259). McKenna (2015, p.40) postulates ‘that the children of victims are at an increased risk of experiencing toxic stress’: which has been defined by Shonkoff et al. (2009, p.360) as the ‘strong, frequent, and/or prolonged activation of the body’s stress response’.
Kellerman (2001) puts forward four major theoretical models for the transmission of TGT: Psychodynamic; Sociocultural; Family Systems; and Biological. Downes et al. (2012, pp. 584-586), who researched families bereaved during the conflict in Northern Ireland, summarise similar approaches including: stress-vulnerability models (in which ‘increased vulnerability to develop psychopathology is [...] transmitted’ [emphasis in the original]); transmission of psychopathology (whereby ‘the parental trauma in itself is not transmitted, but rather that the parental psychopathology is the factor that influences the children’ [emphasis in the original]); genetic and physiological explanations (which posits that the stressor is ‘transmitted physiologically’ to the offspring); social psychological/parenting perspectives (which focuses on ‘social learning and effects on parenting’); psychodynamic theories (through transposition; overidentification; role reversal; and attachment problems); and family system approaches (with a focus on ‘particular interpersonal patterns present in families’). Hanna et al. (2012) encapsulate a phenomenon of poor psychological functioning in children which may stem from the impact of trauma experienced by their caregivers, leading to a deficit in optimal interactions.
While some of these models may be discrete, others are more intertwined and connected. Kellerman (2001) argues that any or all of the manifestations of trauma transmission can be determinants. Weingarten (2004, p.49) echoes this line stating that ‘no mechanism alone is the answer to how trauma passes; none is incontrovertible; and none can be easily separated from the others’ [emphasis in the original].
The psychodynamic model of transmission, according to Kellerman (2001, p. 260), comes from the psychoanalytic tradition where ‘repressed, [unresolved], and insufficiently dealt with’ emotions within the primary victims are passed over to the next generation through unconscious ‘absorption’: which Hesse and Van IJzendoorn (1998, p.304) purport as having the potential to lead to ‘substantial alterations in consciousness’. According to Volkan (1997, in Kellerman, 2001, p.260) the elder ‘unconsciously externalizes his traumatized self onto the developing child’s personality’: an influence that the child cannot repel; thus it becomes the child’s ‘task, [to] mourn, to reverse the humiliation [and shame]’ [emphasis added]. Lev-Wiesel (2007, p.90) also recognises this dynamic, in that, the 2nd generation were expected to repay a ‘debt […] for the suffering of the 1st generation [Holocaust survivors]’: it became their life’s ‘mission’. A recent full page advertisement from a local victim-centred NGO in one of the main newspapers in Northern Ireland (The Irish News) echoes this mission mentality being placed upon subsequent generations, to perpetually seek truth and justice for human rights violations, by stating that, ‘[The UK] Government is waiting for us to die off but our families will not go away’ (News Letter, 2015).
Rowland-Klein and Dunlop’s (1997, p. 366-367) Holocaust research identifies this ‘intimate, intrapsychic, and unconscious’ process in the form of ‘projective identification’: in which the parents attempt to self-heal through the children, who reciprocate ‘despite the cost to themselves’; and ‘whereby the parent splits off the unwanted part of the self, which is projected into the child, and internalised by it; becoming [Kleinian] ‘containers’ for the parent [emphasis added]. DeGraaf (1998, in Downes et al., 2012, p. 585) describes a ’bad child’ within the parent being externalised through ‘anger, rage, sadness disappointment and grief onto their child’ [emphasis added]. Themes that arose in Rowland-Klein and Dunlop’s (1997, pp. 366-367) research included ‘overidentification’ and ‘re-enactment’ which involved the child placing itself in an analogous situation to the parent where they would try to find meaning and ‘share the suffering’, in a process of ‘introjection’ and ‘transposition’.
Overidentification and overprotectiveness are evident when children display similar traits to the parents such as hypervigilance and a pervasive mistrust of others; in which the world is a dangerous place and the family is the only safe haven (Danieli, 1985). Rowland-Klein and Dunlop, (1997, p.367) posit that this can compromise the child’s ‘own sense of security’. This observation is corroborated by Black’s (2004, p.104) research into the children of police officers in Northern Ireland, in which ‘[the children] may develop anxiety regarding their parent’s or their own safety’. The child becomes enmeshed in the reality of the parent in a ‘symbiotic’ relationship; boundaries are blurred; role reversal occurs (‘parental child [vs] adult child’); potentially leaving the child with problems around ‘separation’, ‘individuation’ and ‘autonomy’ (Freyberg, 1980, p.90; Rowland-Klein and Dunlop, 1997, p.366; Downes et al., 2012, p.593). This type of relationship can be detrimental to both parent and child as they may become entwined in what Karpman (1968) has described as ‘The Drama Triangle’.
Kellerman (2001, p.261) shone a light on the ‘sociocultural and socialization models of transmission’ in which it is postulated that social norms and beliefs are passed down from ‘generation to generation’; whereby children ‘form their own images through their parents’ childrearing behaviour’. This contrasts with psychodynamic theories of the unconscious to more direct and conscious learning. In Holocaust literature, according to Kellerman (2001, p.261) survivor parents have been described as ‘inadequate’: as their extreme suffering was ‘assumed to create child-rearing problems around both attachment and detachment’. Difficulties with attachment have been posited as a prominent conceptual framework for the transmission of TGT (Liotti, 1992; Bar-on et al., 1998; Kellerman, 2001; Downes et al., 2012; McKenna, 2015). Attachment is a psychological model which describes the bonds and interactions between a young child and its primary caregiver and it is believed to be an important determinant of the child’s successful social and emotional development (Commission for Victims and Survivors, 2015, p.23). Bowlby (1982) describes how we are born with a strong tendency to seek care, help, and comfort in times of danger or when suffering from physical or emotional stress. Bar-on et al. (1998, p.318) assert that ‘[...] a primary function of attachment relationships is to serve as a source of security [...] in situations that induce fear or anxiety’. Ideally, in an optimal relationship, the caregiver gives a positive response to the child: problems arise when a negative response is offered. These problems may manifest in children as ‘disorganised’ or ‘insecure-ambivalent and preoccupied attachment strateg[ies]’, which could lead to an increased vulnerability to ‘dissociative disorders’ (Liotti, 1992, p.196; Bar-on et al., 1998, p.330).
McKenna (2015, p.37) asserts that ‘parental trauma exposures interfere with interaction patterns within families’. Bar-on et al. (1998, p.319) postulate that ‘parents who are unable to monitor their discourse and thoughts [around traumatic experiences] appear to have children who show a lack of consistent attachment strategy’; adding that it is the ‘lack of successful coping, rather than the loss/trauma per se that contributed to this relation’. Main and Hesse (1990, in Bar-on et al., 1998, p.320) propose that this ‘lack of resolution [...] is characterised by parental fear [...] perceived by the child as being either a frightened model or as directly frightening the child’; and that the attachment figure is ‘at once the source and the solution of the infant’s alarm, and this leads to a paradox of fright without solution’.
McKenna (2015) posits that communication within families can be a detrimental determinant of TGT transmission. McNally (2014, p.32) asserted that traumatic experiences could lead to the development of ‘unhealthy methods of communication’ within the family: ranging ‘from silence to intrusive attempts to discuss the events and imposing their interpretations [onto their children]’. This type of communication can be either intentional or unintentional: verbal or non-verbal (McNally, 2014). Bergman and Jucovy (1982) signal the importance of how the narrative of the trauma is relayed to children, in what spirit, or whether the information is used to educate or employed as a threat.
Danieli (1985, p.298) highlights this ‘Conspiracy of Silence’ as a major factor in the transmission of TGT. The problems with a frightening narrative explained above are seen in contrast to how silence around trauma can be even more insidious to the next generation. Many of the families who survived trauma employed silence as a way of coping with their extreme experiences; and also to protect their children. Their Holocaust experiences were too horrifying to recount; there were no words; they were dealing with feelings of shame and guilt; and at the same time their audience could not or would not listen to or believe them: which led to problems with intrapsychic integration and healing (Ibid.). Nevertheless, many of their offspring ‘attested to the constant psychological presence of the Holocaust at home, verbally and nonverbally [and] reported having absorbed the omnipresent experience [...] through osmosis’ (Ibid., p.299).
However, Bar-on et al., (1998, p.331) stress that the conspiracy of silence ‘cannot be total’. The offspring may hear partial facts and use their imagination to complete the narrative which may result in a more pervasive interpretation; where the ‘made-up story may be even more frightening than the real one’ (Dekel and Goldblatt, 2008, p.285). They are left with an emotional story without an actual narrative to make sense of it. Bar-on (1995, in Bar-on et al., 1998, p.326) asserted that, ‘the “untold story” of the past was [transmitted] with greater intensity [...] than the “told” story’; and that the children became sensitive to their parents’ need to keep silent responding with a “double wall”: [t]he parents did not tell and the children did not ask’.
Danieli (1985, pp.299-304) described four categories of post-Holocaust survivor families to give a sense of the systems they employed: (1) Victim families (characterised by a victim identity, pervasive depression, worry, mistrust, fear of the outside world, and symbiotic clinging); (2) Fighter families (characterised by an ‘intense drive to build and achieve’, mistrust, overinvolvement and overprotectiveness, contemptuous of dependency on others); (3) Numb families (characterised by ‘pervasive silence and depletion of all emotions’, role reversal, children grew up on their own and taught themselves how to live); an observation supported by Becker and Diaz (1998) who argue that such children had to mature rapidly in order to become protectors of their parents; and (4) Families of ‘those who made it’ (characterised by a ‘desire to make it big’, children were emotionally neglected, denial of impact of Holocaust). All of these families have a commonality in that the past is ‘taboo, excluded from open discussion, [which] cannot then become [an integrated] history (Becker and Weyermann (2006, p.28).
Downes et al. (2012, pp.590-595) located this theme in their research of families affected by the NI conflict: whereby families hid ‘aspects of the traumatic experiences and engage[d] in cognitive and affective avoidance’; where the ‘unspoken’ was prevalent; and the children ‘invented [their] own explanations; a place where the ‘truth [was] dangerous’; where facts were ‘hidden but not hidden’; where the children attempted to ‘block out their feeling for fear of the consequences; upon which one child developed the ‘propensity to be the “good girl” [in] a conscious attempt to avoid causing her own mother any more stress’. This echoes Dekel and Goldblatt’s (2008, p.285) observations where ‘sensitive subjects are avoided’ to prevent the intensification of stress.
In conclusion, it becomes clear from the evidence that there is a case to be made for the existence of mechanisms which may pass trauma from one generation to the next; and in certain cases to subsequent unborn generations. However, they are not mutually exclusive and they may not always pass on pathological consequences. There can be recovery; missions can be accomplished; and growth can occur.
The main focus of this essay was concerned with transmission inside families however wider society and political responses can have a profound effect on traumatic sequelae (Becker, 2004; McNally, 2014). From this comes a view that trauma is a biopsychosocial issue that should not be seen through a purely pathological diagnostic lens; ‘in which the basic focus is not so much on the symptoms […] but on the sequential development of the traumatic situation’ (Becker, 2004, p.6). This point is echoed by McNally (2014) who argues that TGT should not be given a clear diagnostic criterion as this may steer any responses firmly in the direction of the medical model to the detriment of other treatment modalities, and of the individuals and families themselves. Until the wider issues, that pervade societies affected by protracted conflict, violence, and trauma, are addressed the chances of seriously dealing with the long term legacy of traumatised families are diminished.
American Psychiatric Association. (2013) Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 5th edn. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Publishing.
Auerhahn, N.C. and Laub, D. (1998) ‘Intergenerational Memory of the Holocaust’, in Danieli, Y. (ed.) International Handbook of Multigenerational Legacies of Trauma. New York: Plenum Press, pp. 21-41.
Bar-On, D. (1995) Fear and Hope: Three Generations of the Holocaust. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Bar-On, D., Eland, J., Kleber, R.J., Krell, R., Moore, Y., and Sagi, A. (1998) Multigenerational perspectives on coping with the Holocaust experience: an attachment perspective for understanding the developmental sequalae of trauma across generations’, International Journal of Behavioural Development, 22(2), pp. 315-338.
Becker, D., and Diaz, M. (1998) ‘The Social Process and the Transgenerational Transmission of Trauma in Chile’, in Danieli, Y. (ed.) International Handbook of Multigenerational Legacies of Trauma. New York: Plenum Press, pp. 435- 445.
Becker, D. and Weyermann, B. (2006) Toolkit: Gender, Conflict Transformation and the Psychosocial Approach. Bern: Swiss Development Co-operation.
Bergmann, M.S and Jucovy, M.E. (eds.) (1982) Generations of the Holocaust. New York: Basic Books.
Black, A. (2004) ‘The treatment of psychological problems experienced by the children of police officers in Northern Ireland’, Child Care in Practice, 10(2), pp. 99-106.
Bowlby, J. (1982) Attachment and loss. Vol.1: Attachment. 2nd edn. London: Hogarth Press.
Commission for Victims and Survivors (2015) Towards A Better Future: The Trans-generational Impact of the Troubles on Mental Health. Belfast: Commission for Victims and Survivors.
Danieli, Y. (1985) ‘The Treatment and Prevention of Long-term Effects and Intergenerational Transmission of Victimization: A Lesson From Holocaust Survivors and Their Children’, in Figley, C.R. (ed.) Trauma and its wake. Vol.1, The study and treatment of post-traumatic stress disorder. New York: Brunner/Mazel, pp. 295-313.
DeGraaf, T. (1998) ‘A family therapeutic approach to transgenerational traumatization’, Family Process, 37(2), pp. 233–243.
Dekel, R. and Goldblatt, H. (2008) ‘Is There Intergenerational Transmission of Trauma? The Case of Combat Veterans’ Children’, American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 78(3), pp. 281–289.
Downes, C., Harrison, E., Curran, D., & Kavanagh, M. (2013) ‘The trauma still goes on...: the multigenerational legacy of Northern Ireland's conflict’, Clinical Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 18(4), pp. 583-603.
Douglass, A. and Vogler, T.A. (eds.) (2003) Witness and Memory: The Discourse of Trauma. Routledge, London.
Freyberg, J.T. (1980) ‘Difficulties in Separation-Individuation as experienced by offspring of Nazi Holocaust Survivors’, American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 50(1), pp. 87-95.
Hanna, D., Dempster, M., Dyer, K., Lyons, E. and Devaney, L. (2012) Young People's Transgenerational Issues in Northern Ireland. Belfast: Commission for Victims and Survivors.
Herman, J.L. (2001) Trauma and recovery: from domestic abuse to political terror. London: Pandora.
Hesse, E. and Van IJzendoorn, M.H. (1998) ‘Parental loss of close family members and propensities towards absorption in offspring’, Developmental Science, 1(2), pp.299-305.
Karpman, S. (1968) ‘Fairy tales and script drama analysis’, Transactional Analysis Bulletin, 7(26), pp. 39-43.
Kellermann, N.P.F. (2001) ‘Transmission of Holocaust Trauma. An integrative view’, Psychiatry: Interpersonal and Biological Processes, 64, pp.256–267.
Laing, R.D. (1971) The Politics of the Family and Other Essays. London: Tavistock Publications.
Lev-Wiesel, R. (2007) ‘Intergenerational Transmission of Trauma across Three Generations: A Preliminary Study’, Qualitative Social Work, 6(1), pp. 75-94.
Liotti, G. (1992) ‘Disorganized/disoriented attachments in the etiology of the dissociative disorders’, Dissociation, 5, pp. 196-204. Main, M. and Hesse, E. (1990) ‘Parents' unresolved traumatic experiences are related to infant 34 disorganized attachment status: Is frightened and/or frightening parental behavior the linking mechanism?’, in Greenberg, M.T., Cicchetti, D. and E.M. Cummings, E.M. (eds.) Attachment in the preschool years. Chicago: Chicago University Press, pp. 161-182.
McKenna, A. (2015) ‘The impact of the conflict’s legacy on early years’ development of children and young people’, in Commission for Victims and Survivors, Towards A Better Future: The Trans-generational Impact of the Troubles on Mental Health. Belfast: Commission for Victims and Survivors, pp. 36-49.
McNally, D, (2014) Transgenerational Trauma and Dealing with the Past in Northern Ireland. Belfast: WAVE Trauma Centre.
News Letter (2015) ‘Troubles victims use newspaper ad to attack Government over legacy issues’, [Online] Available at: http://www.newsletter.co.uk/news/northern-ireland-news/troubles-victims-use-newspaper-ad-to-attack-goverment-over-legacy-issues-1-7108985 (Accessed: 11 December 2015).
Rowland-Klein and Dunlop’s (1997) ‘The transmission of trauma across generations: Identification with parental trauma in children of Holocaust survivors’, Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 32(3), pp. 358-369.
Shonkoff, J.P. (2010) ‘Building a New Biodevelopmental Framework to Guide the Future of Early Childhood Policy’, Child Development, 81(1), pp.357-367.
Summerfield, D. (2000) ‘Conflict and health - War and mental health: a brief overview’, BMJ, 321, pp. 232–235.
Tedeschi, R.G. & Calhoun, L.G. (1996). ‘The posttraumatic growth inventory: measuring the positive legacy of trauma’, Journal of Traumatic Stress, 9(3), pp. 455-471.
Van der Kolk, B. and McFarlane, A.C. (1996) ‘The Black Hole of Trauma’, in Van der Kolk, B., McFarlane, A.C. and Weisaeth, L. (eds.) Traumatic Stress: The Effects of Overwhelming Experience on Mind, Body, and Society. New York, NY: Guilford Press, pp. 3-23.
Volkan, V. (1997) Bloodlines: From Ethnic Pride to Ethnic Terrorism. New York: Basic Books.
Weingarten, K. (2004) ‘Witnessing the effects of political violence in families: Mechanisms of intergenerational transmission of trauma and clinical interventions’, Journal of Marital and Family Therapy, 30(1), pp. 45-59.